The Nature of Race

(Version 1.01)

The Nature of Race
Karl Boetel and John Fuerst

(An online bibliography can be found at the “The Nature of Race” website.)

Introduction
I. Biology — Philosophical Clarifications
I-A. Existing Views: Confusion Abounds
I-B. Biological Concepts in General
I-C. The Validity of Biological Concepts
I-D. Biological Kinds
I-E. Natural Biological Populations
I-F. The Natural Population as a Taxonomic Unit
I-G. Evolutionary Classification, Natural Population, and Sub-specific Variation
I-H. Biologically Meaningful Race Concepts
I-I. Concepts Versus the Things Which They Describe
I-J. Biological Reality
I-K. Biologically Meaningful Race Differences
I-L. Concepts of Biological Race

II. The Evolutionary Race Concept
II-A. The Ordinary Biological Race Concept and the Evolutionary Concept of Race
II-B. What Evolutionary Biological Race Does Not Represent
II-C. Races, Clines, and Clusters?
II-D. Clarification on the Meaning of “Arbitrary” and “Objective” in Context to Population Delineations
II-E. The Semantic Objection
II-F. Different Concepts of Biological Race
II-G. The Relationship between the Evolutionary Race Concept and Other Race Concepts and a Set Conception of Evolutionary Races
II-H. Clarification of the Statement “More Related by Descent”
II-I. Estimating Composite DNA Similarity
II-J. Mixed and Undifferentiated Individuals
II-K. Essential, Cluster, and Fuzzy Sets and Back to Populations
II-L. Sociological Clarifications

III. The Ontology of Biological Race
III-A. Other Defenses of Biological Race
III-B. Biological Races, Biological Reality, and Social Constructs
III-C. Tilting Against What?
III-D. Weak Biological Essentialism
III-E. A Defense of Racial Essentialism (in the Explanatory Sense)
III-F. Are Thick Races Defensible? Yes

IV. The Races of Man
IV-A. A very Brief Historical Review
IV-B. Human Biological Races and Scientific Consensus
IV-C. Racial Classifications and Biological Race Concepts
IV-D. Human Holocene Races
IV-E. HHR as Biologically objective Races
IV-F. HHR and Migration and Miscegenation
IV-G. HHR and Subspecies
IV-H. HRR and Cluster Discordance
IV-I. HHR and Taxonomy: Does the TRCL Represent a Valid Evolutionary Classification Scheme?
IV-J. “Significant” Racial Differences
IV-K. Shades of de Gobineau
IV-L. Race and Intelligence

V. Critique of Anti-biological Race Arguments
V-A. Anti-Biological Race Arguments
V-B. Biological Arguments
V-B.1. Subspecies Arguments
V-B.2. Panmixia Arguments
V-B.3. Population Structure Arguments
V-C.1. Bio-statistical Arguments: Independent Variation
V-C.2. Bio-statistical Arguments: Genetic Differentiation
V-D. Sociological Arguments
V-E.1. Super-Naturalistic Arguments
V-E.2. The Numbers Game
V-F. Semantic Arguments
V-G. True Race Arguments
V-G.1. Races Were Histroically Thought of as Subspecies Argument
V-G.2. Races Were Histroically Thought of as Sharply Discontinuous Argument
V-H. Moral Arguments

Conclusion
Post Script

Appendix 1. Was Darwin an Evolutionary Classifier?
References

GED scores by Ethnicity and Nation

I took a gander at the 2010, 2011, and 2012 GED total scores by race and nation (from “GED Testing Statistical Reports”). The sample sizes were small. Unfortunately, the earlier reports, which go back to the ’80s didn’t provide scores for Bermuda, the Virgin Islands, and Jamaica+Cayman+St.Martin; as these scores were what I was particularly curious about, I didn’t include scores from earlier years. The scores aren’t representative, etc., etc. but they, nonetheless, provide a tad of info on e.g., (self-identified) ethnic differences in Bermuda. The score were averaged across the three years mentioned. The d-values presented at the bottom are inter-national. Those presented on the right are intra-national. The differences are roughly consistent with Richard Lynn’s Global Bell Curve position.

GEDRACENATION

Excel.

L&V’s (2012) National IQs predict 2011-2012 GRE scores for 114 citizenship groups, 2010 + 2012 TOEFL scores for 157 citizenship groups, PISA scores of migrants from 62 nations of origin across 17 destination nations, 19th century (birth cohort 1820) numeracy rates across 54 nations, and early 20th century (birth cohort 1890) numeracy across 129 nations

MH’s (02/11/2014) Excel File Here.

Previously, we looked at the association between L&V’s (2012) National IQs, GMAT scores, and English Proficiency scores. We extend that analysis here by including 2010-2012 GRE (quantitative, verbal, and total) scores, 2010 + 2012 TOEFL scores, 2003-2009 migrant PISA scores, and national numeracy rates from the 19th and early 20th century.

Continue reading

Quick Post: L&V’s National IQs predict GMAT scores across 173 nations

Introduction

The GMAT is a graduate entrance test used by more than 5,900 business programs offered by more than 2,100 universities worldwide. While the test is given in English, it is designed to be as minimally English dependent as necessary to predict successful completion of Business programs taught in English. Further, the test is carefully scrutinized for item bias. Rudner (2012) explains:

Yes, the GMAT test is administered in English and is designed for programs that teach in English. But the required English skill level is much less than what students will need in the classroom. The exam requires just enough English to allow us to adequately and comprehensively assess Verbal reasoning, Quantitative reasoning and Integrated Reasoning skills….

We carefully review our questions using criteria defining good item construction. We also compute statistics to assess whether our questions are appropriate across culture groups. We constantly update guidelines for our item writers, including a master list of terms and phrases to avoid in order to assure cultural fairness. By using carefully defined and thorough item development and review processes, along with statistical analyses to flag questions with possible cultural bias, we have developed a test that minimizes the impact of culture and language. The GMAT exam is the best objective measure of the likelihood of success in management programs across the globe.

Despite the claimed lack of bias and apparent predictive validity of the test, there is substantial global variance in scores. Rudner (2012) attributes this variance largely to differences in native language spoken and to differences in self-selection.

We decided to explore to what extent global differences could be accounted for differences in National IQ. To do this, we examined the relation between measures of national cognitive ability, English language proficiency, English language usage, and GMAT scores by reported citizenship. We also sought to determine to what extent GMAT scores could be used to index the National IQs for poorly investigated regions such as North Korea, Rwanda, and St. Kitts.

Continue reading

Structural relationship between g, parental SES, and Achievement : Investigation of the g nexus

The present analysis, using the NLSY97, attempts to model the structural relationship between the latent second-order g factor extracted from the 12 ASVAB subtests, the parental SES latent factor from 3 indicators of parental SES, and the GPA latent factor from 5 domains of grade point averages. A structural equation modeling (SEM) bootstrapping approach combined with a Predictive Mean Matching (PMM) multiple imputation has been employed. The structural path from parental SES to GPA, independently of g, appears to be trivial in the black, hispanic, and white population. The analysis is repeated for the 3 ACT subtests, yielding an ACT-g latent factor. The same conclusion is observed. Most of the effect of SES on GPA appears to be mediated by g. Adding grade variable substantially increases the contribution of parental SES on the achievement factor, which was partially mediated by g. Missing data is handled with PMM multiple imputation. Univariate and multivariate normality tests are carried out in SPSS and AMOS, and through bootstrapping. Full result provided in EXCEL at the end of the article.

Continue reading

Racial Differences on Digit Span Tests

In digit span tests, the respondents are asked to repeat a string of digits. There are two variants of the test, forward digit span (FDS) and backward digit span (BDS). In FDS, the digits are repeated in the order of their presentation, while in BDS they must be repeated in the reverse order. The largest number of digits that a person can repeat without error is his or her forward or backward digit span.

It is well-established that the black-white gap is substantially larger on BDS than FSD (see references in The g Factor by Jensen, p. 405, Note 22; see also my recent analysis of the DAS-II). However, replication is always good, so I analyzed black-white differences in the CNLSY sample, which contains FDS and BDS scores for relatively large samples of black and white children. Additionally, I compared the digit span performance of Hispanic American children to that of blacks and whites. Continue reading

Spearman’s Hypothesis and Racial Differences on the DAS-II

According to Spearman’s hypothesis, the magnitude of the black-white gap on a given cognitive ability test is primarily determined by the test’s g loading. Tests that are better measures of g are associated with larger gaps.

The Differential Ability Scales, Second Edition, or the DAS-II, is an IQ test for assessing children and adolescents. It comprises a total of 21 subtests, although in the present analysis only 13 subtests are used, because not all tests are administered across age groups. I will use the method of correlated vectors (MCV) to test whether g loadings are correlated with mean racial differences on the DAS-II subtests. In addition to the black-white gap, I will also investigate if the test performance of Asians and Hispanics is predicted by g loadings. Continue reading