It’s been almost 50 years now that the famous study of Willerman et al. (1974) has been published. This study is regularly cited as one of the most convincing evidence against the hereditarian hypothesis, despite strong emphasis by hereditarians on the failure of experimental efforts to raise IQ (more specifically, g) and population differences magnifying during adolescence or adulthood due to increasing heritability with age (Jensen, 1998, pp. 333-344, 359, 474; See Malloy [2013] for a case of a stability model with respect to the Black-White gap). Caution about this study is now vindicated. The data used by Willerman also revealed a pattern: the IQ deficits related to having a Black mother seem to vanish over time (Hu, 2022).

When Willerman et al. (1974) analyzed the Collaborative Perinatal Project data, they reported two findings. First, that interracial infants tested at 8 month of age did not show any deficits related to having a Black mother (versus White mother). Second, that interracial children of Black mother tested at age 4 did show a large IQ deficit. The “race of mother” hypothesis was then proposed as an explanation for this outcome, as mother is the primary agent of socialization in the family. Accordingly, adequate parenting would produce huge effect.

Extensive research has been conducted on the effect of education and parenting on cognitive ability. Often, when meaningful gains were found at young ages, they usually fade out several years later (Besharov et al. 2011; te Nijenhuis et al., 2014, 2015). Education programs such as Head Start, Abecedarian, etc., failed at raising IQ permanently despite training the mothers and providing extensive socialization services. But the argument already falls flat to begin with if parenting doesn’t have a lasting effect on cognitive ability (Beaver et al., 2014). Jensen’s argument about the limits of environmental variations is summarized in Dalliard’s modern restatement of Jensen’s model.

The one peculiarity with Willerman’s proposition however is that parenting effect (as indexed by mother’s race in their situation) affects children’s cognitive ability above and beyond socioeconomic status, since their study controlled for socioeconomic status. This could be a weird way of conducting the analysis because (good) parenting should be correlated with socioeconomic status. Moreover, environmental variables should not even be treated as purely environmental.

While it is correct to say that including socioeconomic status (SES) as covariate will partial out some of the genetically-induced parental characteristics and therefore lacks theoretical sense, directly testing their hypothesis requires the inclusion of SES as a covariate in a regression.

Therefore, in my recent paper, published at OpenPsych, I re-analyzed the CPP data since IQ scores at age 7 were available and Willerman only reported IQ scores at age 4. After controlling for SES, the IQ gap at age 4 was as high as 7 points but dropped at 4 points at age 7. Without controlling for SES, the IQ gap is similar at both ages. Whether the gap would be further reduced later is unknown but it wouldn’t be surprising. In the paper, I mentioned that the gap reduction is mostly driven by married women, since children of unmarried women didn’t reduce the gap. However, as I noted here, this is due to sampling error.

However, to fully test the “race of mother” hypothesis, one needs to consider other ethnic groups as well. After analyzing the children of Hispanic-White matings and the children of Asian-White matings, I couldn’t confirm the hypothesis again. Even the opposite was true from the Hispanic group. If Hispanic mother is suspected to be associated with lower children IQ, the CPP showed that children with Hispanic mothers (as opposed to White mothers) had much higher IQ, before and after controlling for SES.

After analyzing the CPP, I turned to the Add Health and the HSLS. While the Add Health collected verbal IQ (AHPVT) scores, the HSLS instead collected math assessments. I wouldn’t consider the latter a true cognitive test.

If we consider the Add Health, the AHPVT scores for Wave I and Wave III showed a pattern which contradicts the race of mother hypothesis. If Asian mother is suspected to be associated with higher children verbal score, it wasn’t evidenced in this data since White mothers had children with a 9 point advantage at Wave I which then was reduced to only 3 point advantage at Wave III. In fact, only the Hispanic-White mating group supported the hypothesis (but this will be explained below).

If we consider finally the HSLS, the math scores still contradict the race of mother hypothesis. Mother’s race wasn’t strongly related to math scores among Hispanic-White and Asian-White matings. But among Black-White mating, Black mothers had children with a large score advantage.

To complement the above findings, I conducted a separate multiple regression for each ethnic group, controlling for SES. After which, a meta-analysis was conducted in order to combine the regression betas across data. The result is displayed in Table 10 (click to enlarge).

The above figure displays a forest plot of each individual effects within each study. Each one of these estimates is weak and has a value close to zero. The weaker data of the bunch is the Add Health, likely because the sample size is comparatively smaller (due to the public data set having smaller sample than the private one).

Because the meta-analysis was performed using regression betas after partialling out for SES, it could be said that the betas aren’t comparable due to covariates varying qualitatively across datasets, but only parental education was used, and since the variable is very similar across datasets, there isn’t much concern. The advantage of this analysis is that, now, it is easy to further extend it in the future by adding more datasets, since the beta and SE estimates are all available in the spreadsheet (available at OSF). While the meta-analysis looks compelling, I wouldn’t put too much attention to it (yet). One intriguing pattern I found across all these datasets is the fact that children cognitive score seems to closely follow the parents’ characteristics. This isn’t always consistent (small data sets and different tests don’t help much here) but still is worth observing.

Consider this. In the CPP, White mother was positively associated with children IQ among Black-White mating, but White mothers had higher education than Black mothers. In the HSLS, Black mother was positively associated with children score while they also had higher education than White mothers. In the CPP, White mother was negatively associated with children IQ among Hispanic-White mating while they also had lower education than Hispanic mothers. But in the Add Health, Hispanic mother had lower education and also was negatively associated with children verbal score (before controlling for SES). Although I mentioned mother’s education here, I am not implying that father’s education isn’t related to children score or is less important than the mother’s, and I find little evidence to this claim.

One challenge interpreting the data well is that SES may have a different impact across ethnic groups. For instance, if SES explains a huge portion of the Hispanic-White gap but only partially explains the Black-White gap, as is usually the case, it means additional factors have to be considered with respect to the Black-White cognitive gap.

References

  • Beaver, K. M., Schwartz, J. A., Al-Ghamdi, M. S., Kobeisy, A. N., Dunkel, C. S., & van der Linden, D. (2014). A closer look at the role of parenting-related influences on verbal intelligence over the life course: Results from an adoption-based research design. Intelligence, 46, 179–187.
  • Besharov, D. J., Germanis, P., Higney, C. A., & Call, D. M. (2011). Assessing the evaluations of early childhood education programs. Welfare Reform Academy. Retrieved from http://www.welfareacademy.org/pubs/early_education/index.shtml
  • Hu, M. (2022). Does Mother’s Involvement Matter for The Cognitive Development of Interracial Children? Testing the Race of the Mother Hypothesis. OpenPsych.
  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor and the design of education. Intelligence, instruction, and assessment: Theory into practice, 111–131.
  • te Nijenhuis, J., Jongeneel-Grimen, B., & Kirkegaard, E. O. (2014). Are headstart gains on the g factor? a meta-analysis. Intelligence, 46, 209-215.
  • te Nijenhuis, J., Jongeneel-Grimen, B., & Armstrong, E. L. (2015). Are adoption gains on the g factor? a meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 56-60.
  • Willerman, L., Naylor, A. F., & Myrianthopoulos, N. C. (1974). Intellectual development of children from interracial matings: Performance in infancy and at 4 years. Behavior Genetics, 4(1), 83–90.